Data Sources
D-PLACE contains cultural, linguistic, environmental and geographic information
for over 2000 human cultural groups. This data is aggregated from a number of
datasets.
Cultural data
⇫
D-PLACE aggregates cultural data from several major cross-cultural datasets.
All datasets use codes to characterize the cultural practices of a ‘society’, or group of people with a
shared language and cultural identity at a given location and point in time.
All cultural descriptions are tagged with the date to which they refer, a geographic location
(using a reported latitude and longitude) and language. This allows users to simultaneously consider
how cultural practices relate to linguistic ancestry, practices of neighbouring groups, and the environment.
The authors of the cultural datasets relied on a huge number of
primary data sources to code cultural practices of societies in their
samples.
Most of these sources are original ethnographies published as academic journal articles or books. While Murdock,
Binford
and their successors carefully documented their sources, the references were for the most part excluded from
early
attempts to digitize the EA and
Binford datasets. Also lost from early digital datasets
were the authors’ comments
regarding particular coding decisions, despite the insights and caveats these comments provide.
In D-PLACE, each cultural data point is tagged with both its primary sources and coding comments, with
references for
the primary sources and comments included in results tables and data downloads. We encourage users to draw on
this
information when considering intracultural variation and uncertainty in cultural codes, and to return to the
primary
sources for a better understanding of particular coding decisions.
In order to facilitate access to supplementary cultural data for D-PLACE societies, we provide information on
whether each society appears in other major cross-cultural databases. We have included links to the
Human Relations Area Files (HRAF) (Murdock 1983), a repository of annotated
primary literature that can be searched by topic e.g., marriage system (see this page on eHRAF and D-PLACE
collaboration).
These links to other cross-cultural datasets appear at the top of individual “Society” results pages. Kirby et
al. (2016) includes a discussion regarding the combination of data from different cross-cultural datasets,
including the importance of considering agreement among focal year and focal location. More thorough discussions
are included in Ember et al. (1992) and Ember (2007).
Linguistic data
⇫
Language family
The language spoken by a society is an important indicator for historical relatedness, cultural identity and
contact. D-PLACE specifies the broad language family affiliation for all societies, using the classification
systems of Glottolog (Hammarström et al. 2023). Users can treat language family as a variable of interest
itself, or can use it as a coarse-level control for relatedness among societies (e.g., Botero et al. 2014).
Historical relationships among languages: Glottolog trees
At a closer resolution, all societies in D-PLACE have been linked to a language and, in cases where the language
was shared with another D-PLACE society, to a Glottolog dialect. Languages are identified by both a Glottocode
and an ISO 639-3 code, and dialects by a Glottocode (Hammarström et al. 2023; SIL International 2015).
Languages and dialects are used by D-PLACE to link
each society to Glottolog’s language classification trees. These trees are topological only, representing
genealogical hypotheses of how languages are nested, based on comparative historical linguistic work. The
classifications are purely taxonomies and branch lengths do not represent time or amount of change.
Distance among languages: phylogenies
At the finest scale, many of the societies in each cross-cultural dataset belong to a language family for which
a well-resolved and computationally-derived phylogenetic tree is available (for example: Gray et al 2009,
Kitchen et al. 2009, Dunn et al. 2011, Lee and Hasegawa 2011, Bowern and Atkinson 2012, Bouckaert et al 2012,
Chacon and List 2015, Grollemund et al. 2015, Sicoli & Holton 2015, Lee 2015). In focusing analyses on these
societies, researchers gain the ability to conduct sophisticated hypothesis testing about evolutionary change
using phylogenetic comparative methods, as well as robust control for historical relatedness. For example, the
relative time since language divergence can be used as a measure of relative distance among societies. Of
course, while language provides a highly effective proxy for shared history, language family affiliation may not
always reflect deep cultural or linguistic ancestry. Numerous instances of language shift, contact, and
borrowing occur when societies interact. For example, many Central African Pygmy groups have adopted the
languages of their Bantu trading partners (Bahuchet 2012). In such cases, linguistic relationships still capture
meaningful aspects of cultural interaction, but users will need to make their own context-specific judgments.
For details on how societies were matched to languages, please see Kirby et al. (2016).
Environmental data
⇫
We sampled environmental variables at the localities reported for each society in each dataset, with some
adjustments to geographic coordinates as outlined in Kirby et al. (2016).
Climate
For each society, we computed the mean, variance, and predictability of the entire annual cycles of
precipitation and temperature based on monthly global maps (0.5 by 0.5 degree cells) obtained from the Baseline
Historical (1900-1949), CCSM ecoClimate model (Lima-Ribeiro,M. et al. 2015). Predictability was measured via
Colwell’s (1974) Constancy, Contingency and Predictability indexes. These indexes capture the extent to which
yearly cycles vary among years in terms of onset, intensity and duration, ranging from 0 (completely
unpredictable) to 1 (fully predictable). We include constancy (the extent to which a variable can be predicted
because it tends to stay fairly constant) and contingency (the extent to which predictions are possible because
environmental cycles are highly repeatable) in order to allow interested users to explore the potentially
different impacts of these two types of predictability. Because the cultural data for the vast majority of
societies in D-PLACE was collected between 1901 and 1950, we sampled climatic variables at each locality for
this particular time period.
Productivity and biodiversity
Ecoregion and biome locations of each society were obtained from Olson et al. (2001). Monthly net primary
production data were obtained from the MODIS dataset (Running et al. 1999, Data range: 2000-2016). From these
data we computed the annual mean, variance predictability, constancy and contingency of net primary productivity
at each sampled locality. Estimates of the number of species at each site were obtained for birds, mammals, and
amphibians from Jenkins et al. (2013) and for vascular plants from Kreft and Jetz (2007).
Physical environment
We also include estimates of distance from a coast, elevation, and slope for all societies, with topographical
data provided by the Global Multi-resolution Terrain Elevation Data of the U.S. Geological Survey.
References
⇫
- Anderson, J. L., Crawford, C. B., Nadeau, J., & Lindberg, T. (1992). Was the Duchess of Windsor right? A
cross-cultural review of the socioecology of ideals of female body shape. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3),
197-227.
- Bahuchet, S. (2012). Changing language, remaining pygmy. Human Biology, 84(1), 11-43.
- Barry, H., & Paxson, L. M. (1971). Infancy and early childhood: Cross-cultural codes 2. Ethnology,
10(4), 466-508.
- Barry, H. (1980). Ethnographic Atlas XXVIII. Ethnology 19(2): 245-263.
- Barry, H., & Schlegel, A. (1982). Cross-cultural codes on contributions by women to subsistence.
Ethnology, 21(2), 165-188.
- Barry, H., & Schlegel, A. (1984). Measurements of adolescent sexual behavior in the standard sample of
societies. Ethnology, 23(4), 315-329.
- Barry, H., Josephson, L., Lauer, E., & Marshall, C. (1976). Traits inculcated in childhood:
Cross-cultural codes 5. Ethnology, 15(1), 83-106.
- Barry, H., Josephson, L., Lauer, E., & Marshall, C. (1977). Agents and techniques for child training:
Cross-cultural codes 6. Ethnology, 16(2), 191-230.
- Betzig, L. (1989). Causes of conjugal dissolution: A cross-cultural study. Current Anthropology, 30(5),
654-676.
- Betzig, L. L. (1986). Despotism and differential reproduction: A Darwinian view of history. Aldine
Publishing Co.
- Binford, L. R. (2001). Constructing frames of reference: an analytical method for archaeological theory
building using ethnographic and environmental data sets. Univ of California Press.
- Binford, L. R., and Johnson, A. L. (2006). Documentation for Program for Calculating Environmental and
Hunter-Gatherer Frames of Reference (ENVCALC2). Java version, August 2006.
- Bondarenko, D., Kazankov, A., Khaltourina, D., and Korotayev, A. (2005). Ethnographic atlas XXXI: Peoples of
easternmost Europe. Ethnology, 261–289.
- Botero, C.A., Gardner, B, Kirby, K.R., Bulbulia, J., Gavin, M.C. and Gray, R.D. (2014) The ecology of
religious beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (47), 16784-16789.
- Bouckaert, R., Lemey, P., Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Alekseyenko, A. V., Drummond, A. J., Gray, R. D.,
Suchard, M. A., and Atkinson, Q. D. (2012). Mapping the Origins and Expansion of the Indo-European Language
Family. Science 337, 957. DOI: 10.1126/science.1219669.
- Bowern, C. (2016). Chirila: Contemporary and Historical Resources for the Indigenous Languages of Australia.
Language Documentation and Conservation. February, 2016. Vol 10: 1-44. http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/ldc/?p=1002.
- Bowern, C., and Atkinson, Q. (2012). Computational phylogenetics and the internal structure of Pama-Nyungan.
Language, 88(4), 817-845.
- Bradley, C. (1994). The Household Division of Work: SCCS Codes. World Cultures, 8(2), 6-40.
- Bradley, C., Moore, C. C., Burton, M. L., & White, D. R. (1990). A Cross?Cultural Historical Analysis of
Subsistence Change. American Anthropologist, 92(2), 447-457.
- Broude, G. J., & Greene, S. J. (1976). Cross-cultural codes on twenty sexual attitudes and practices.
Ethnology, 15(4), 409-429.
- Broude, G. J., & Greene, S. J. (1983). Cross-cultural codes on husband-wife relationships. Ethnology,
22(3), 263-280.
- Burton, M. L. (1999). Language and region codes for the standard cross-cultural sample. Cross-Cultural
Research, 33(1), 63-83.
- Cashdan, E. (2001). Ethnic diversity and its environmental determinants: Effects of climate, pathogens, and
habitat diversity. American Anthropologist, 103(4), 968-991.
- Chacon T. C., and List, J. M. (2015) Improved computational models of sound change shed light on the history
of the Tukanoan languages. Journal of Language Relationship • Вопросы языкового родства.
13:177-203.
- Colwell R. K. (1974) Predictability, constancy, and contingency of periodic phenomena. Ecology. 1:1148-53.
- Dirks, R. (1993). Starvation and famine: cross-cultural codes and some hypothesis tests. Cross-Cultural
Research, 27(1-2), 28-69.
- Divale, W. (1999). Climatic instability, food storage, and the development of numerical counting: A
cross-cultural study. Cross-cultural research, 33(4), 341-368.
- Divale, W. and Seda, A. (2000). Cross-cultural codes of modernization. World Cultures, 11(2), 152-170.
- Divale, W. T., Abrams, N., Barzola, J., Harris, E., & Henry, F. M. (1998). Sleeping arrangements of
children and adolescents: SCCS sample codes. World Cultures, 9(2), 3-12.
- Divale, W., & Seda, A. (1999). Codes on Gossip for Societies in the Standard Sanple. World Cultures,
10(1), 7-22.
- Divale, W., & Seda, A. (1999). Codes on Gossip for Societies in the Standard Sanple. World Cultures,
10(1), 7-22.
- Dunn M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., and Gray, R.D. (2011) Evolved structure of language shows
lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature. 5;473(7345):79-82.
- Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (1992). Warfare, aggression, and resource problems: Cross-cultural codes.
Behavior Science Research, 26(1-4), 169-226.
- Ember, C. R., Page, H., Martin, M. M., and O’Leary, T. (1992). A computerized concordance of cross-cultural
samples. New Haven: Human Relations Area Files.
- Ember, C. R. (2007). Using the HRAF collection of ethnography in conjunction with the Standard
Cross-Cultural Sample and the Ethnographic Atlas. Cross-Cultural Research, 41(4), 396–427.
- Frayser, S. G. (1985). Varieties of sexual experience: An anthropological perspective on human sexuality.
Human Relations Area Files.
- Gray, J. P. (1999). A corrected ethnographic atlas. World Cultures, 10(1), 24–85.
- Gray, R. D., Drummond, A. J., and Greenhill, S. J. (2009). Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and
pauses in Pacific settlement. Science, 323(5913), 479–483.
- Grollemund, R., Branford, S., Bostoen, K., Meade, A., Venditti, C., and Pagel, M. (2015). Bantu expansion
shows that habitat alters the route and pace of human dispersals. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences (PNAS) 112(43), 13296-13301.
- Hammarström, H., Forkel, R., Haspelmath, M., Bank, S. (2015). Glottolog 2.7. Jena: Max Planck Institute for
the Science of Human History. Accessed 2015-2016. URL: http://glottolog.org.
- Jenkins, C. N., Pimm, S. L., and Joppa, L. N. (2013). Global patterns of terrestrial vertebrate diversity
and conservation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(28), E2602–E2610.
- Jorgensen, J. G. (1980). Western Indians: Comparative environments, languages, and cultures of 172 Western
American Indian tribes. San Francisco: WH Freeman. [Original monograph, source of bibliography for Jorgensen
societies that is included in D-PLACE]
- Jorgensen, J. G. (1999a). An empirical procedure for defining and sampling culture bearing units in
continuous geographic areas. World Cultures, 10(2), 139-143. [Describes sample of 172 societies]
- Jorgensen, J. G. (1999b). Codebook for Western Indians Data. World Cultures, 10(2), 144-293. [Defines
codes]
- Kirby, K. R., Gray, R. D., Greenhill, S. J., Jordan, F. M., Gomes-Ng, S. Bibiko, H. J., Blasi, D. E.,
Botero, C. A., Bowern, C., Ember, C. R., Leehr, D., Low, B. S., McCarter, J., Divale, W. and Gavin, M.
(Submitted). D-PLACE: A Global Database of Cultural, Linguistic and Environmental Diversity.
- Kitchen, A., Ehret, C., Assefa, S., and Mulligan, C. J. (2009). Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of Semitic
languages identifies an Early Bronze Age origin of Semitic in the Near East. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London B: Biological Sciences, rspb-2009. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0408.
- Korotayev, A. (2004). World religions and social evolution of the old world Oikumene civilizations: A
cross-cultural perspective. E. Mellen Press.
- Korotayev, A., Kazankov, A., Borinskaya, S., Khaltourina, D., and Bondarenko, D. (2004). Ethnographic atlas
XXX: peoples of Siberia. Ethnology, 83–92.
- Kreft, H., and Jetz, W. (2007). Global patterns and determinants of vascular plant diversity. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, 104(14), 5925–5930.
- Lang, H. (1998). CONAN: An electronic code-text data-base for cross-cultural studies. World Cultures, 9(2),
13-56.
- Lee, S., and Hasegawa, T. (2011). Bayesian phylogenetic analysis supports an agricultural origin of Japonic
languages. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, rspb20110518.
- Lee, S. (2015). A Sketch of Language History in the Korean Peninsula. PloS one, 10(5), e0128448.
- Lima-Ribeiro, M. et al. 2015. ecoClimate: a database of climate data from multiple models for past, present,
and future for Macroecologists and Biogeographers. Biodiversity Informatics 10, 1-21.
- Low, B. S. (1994). Pathogen intensity cross-culturally. World Cultures, 8(1), 24-34.
- Ludvico, L. R. (1994). Scarification, pathogen load and biome: Cross-cultural codes. World Cultures 9(1).
- Murdock, G. P. (1957). World ethnographic sample. American anthropologist, 59(4), 664-687.
- Murdock, G. P. (1962-1971). Ethnographic Atlas, Installments I-XXVII. Ethnology, 1-10.
- Murdock, G. P. (1965). Ethnographic Atlas IVX. Ethnology, 4(2) 241-250.
- Murdock, G. P. (1967) Ethnographic Atlas. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
- Murdock, G. P. and White, D. R. (1969). Standard Cross-Cultural Sample. Ethnology 8: 329-369.
- Murdock, G. P. (1970). Kin term patterns and their distribution. Ethnology, 9(2), 165-208.
- Murdock, G. P., & Morrow, D. O. (1970). Subsistence economy and supportive practices: Cross-cultural
codes 1. Ethnology, 9(3), 302-330.
- Murdock, G. P., & Wilson, S. F. (1972). Settlement patterns and community organization: Cross-cultural
codes 3. Ethnology, 11(3), 254-295.
- Murdock, G. P., & Provost, C. (1973). Factors in the division of labor by sex: A cross-cultural
analysis. Ethnology, 12(2), 203-225.
- Murdock, G. P., & Provost, C. (1973). Measurement of cultural complexity. Ethnology, 12(4), 379-392.
- Murdock, G. P. (1980). Theories of illness: A world survey. University of Pittsburgh Pre.
- Murdock, G. P. (1983). Outline of world cultures. Sixth Edition. New Haven Ct: Human Relations Area Files.
- Murdock, G. P., and White, D. R. (1969). Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology, 329–369.
- Nammour, V. W. (1974). Drums And Guns: A Cross-cultural Study Of The Nature Of War. (Doctoral Dissertation,
University of Oregon).
- Olson, D. M., Dinerstein, E., Wikramanayake, E. D., Burgess, N. D., Powell, G. V. N., Underwood, E. C.,
D'amico, J. A., Itoua, I., Strand, H. E., Morrison, J. C., Loucks, C. J., Allnutt, T. F., Ricketts, T. H.,
Kura, Y., Lamoreux, J. F., Wettengel, W. W., Hedao, P., and Kassem, K. R. (2001). Terrestrial Ecoregions of
the World: A New Map of Life on Earth A new global map of terrestrial ecoregions provides an innovative tool
for conserving biodiversity. BioScience, 51(11), 933–938.
- Paige, K., & Paige, J. M. (1981). The politics of reproductive ritual. Univ of California Press.
- Patterson, O. (1982). Slavery and social death. Harvard University Press.
- Pryor, F. L. (1985). The invention of the plow. Comparative Studies in Society and history, 27(4),
727-743.
- Pryor, F. L. (1986). The adoption of agriculture: Some theoretical and empirical evidence. American
Anthropologist, 88(4), 879-894.
- Roberts, J. M. (1976). Belief in the evil eye in world perspective. In Maloney, C. (Ed.). The evil eye.
Columbia University Press. p 223-278.
- Rohner, R. P., & Rohner, E. C. (1981). Parental acceptance-rejection and parental control:
Cross-cultural codes. Ethnology, 20(3), 245-260.
- Rohner, R. P., & Rohner, E. C. (1982). Enculturative continuity and the importance of caretakers:
Cross-cultural codes. Behavior Science Research, 17(1-2), 91-114.
- Rohner, R. P., Berg, D. S., & Rohner, E. C. (1982). Data quality control in the standard cross-cultural
sample: cross-cultural codes. Ethnology, 21(4), 359-369.
- Rosenblatt, P. C. (1990). Cross-cultural variation in the experience, expression, and understanding of
grief. In Irish, D. P., Lundquist, K. F., Jenkins Nelsen, V. (Eds.). Ethnic variations in dying, death and
grief: Diversity in universality. Taylor & Francis. p. 13-19.
- Rosenblatt, P. C., Walsh, R. P., & Jackson, D. A. (2011). Grief and mourning codes. World Cultures
eJournal, 18(2).
- Ross, M. H. (1983). Political decision making and conflict: Additional cross-cultural codes and scales.
Ethnology, 22(2), 169-192.
- Rozée-Koker, P. D. (1987). Cross-cultural codes on seven types of rape. Behavior Science Research, 21(1-4),
101-117.
- Running, S. W., Nemani, R., Glassy, J. M., and Thornton, P. E. (1999). MODIS daily photosynthesis (PSN) and
annual net primary production (NPP) product (MOD17) Algorithm Theoretical Basis Document. University of
Montana, SCF At-Launch Algorithm ATBD Documents (available online at: www. ntsg. umt.
edu/modis/ATBD/ATBD_MOD17_v21. pdf).
- Sanday, P. R. (1981). Female power and male dominance: On the origins of sexual inequality. Cambridge
University Press.
- Schlegel, A., & Barry, H. (1979). Adolescent initiation ceremonies: A cross-cultural code. Ethnology,
18(2), 199-210.
- Schlegel, A., & Eloul, R. (1987). A new coding of marriage transactions. Cross-Cultural Research,
21(1-4), 118-140.
- Schroeder, S. (2001). Secondary disposal of the dead: cross-cultural codes. World Cultures, 12(1), 77-93.
- Sicoli, M. A., and Holton, G. (2014). Linguistic phylogenies support back-migration from Beringia to Asia.
PloS one, 9(3), e91722.
- SIL International. (2015). ISO 639-3 Registration Authority. URL: http://www-01.sil.org/iso639-3.
- Tuden, A., & Marshall, C. (1972). Political organization: Cross-cultural codes 4. Ethnology, 11(4),
436-464.
- White, D. R. (1986). Forms and frequencies of polygyny: Standard Sample codes. World Cultures 2(2).
- White, D. R. (1989). Female contribution to subsistence: measurement and reliability. World Cultures.
- White, D. R. (1989). Kinship avoidance. World Cultures 5(4).
- White, D. R. (2009). Pinpointing sheets for the standard cross-cultural sample: complete edition. World
Cultures eJournal, 17(1).
- White, D. R., & Burton, M. L. (1988). Causes of polygyny: Ecology, economy, kinship, and warfare.
American Anthropologist, 90(4), 871-887.
- Whiting, J. W., Sodergren, J. A., & Stigler, S. M. (1982). Winter temperature as a constraint to the
migration of preindustrial peoples. American Anthropologist, 84(2), 279-298.
- Whyte, M. K. (1978). Cross-cultural codes dealing with the relative status of women. Ethnology, 17(2),
211-237.
- Whyte, M. K. (2015). The status of women in preindustrial societies. Princeton University Press.
- Winkelman, M. J. (1984). A cross-cultural study of magico-religious practitioners (Doctoral dissertation,
University of California, Irvine).